---

By: David Bloomberg Re: Making Monsters (File: MAKEMNS3.ZIP) Date: Mon Mar 20 1995 15:24:02 From: SHEPPARD GORDON Subj: False memory & Monsters When memory turns into monsters Richard Ofshe and Ethan Watters make a well-argued case against recovered memory therapy. 2/07/95 SAN FRANCISCO EXAMINER "Making Monsters: False Memories, Psychotherapy, and Sexual Hysteria." Richard Ofshe and Ethan Watters. Scribners. 340 pages. $23. THE CURRENT BATTLE over the veracity of abuse memories recovered in psychotherapy is something of a national morality play. For its defenders, the recovered memory movement reveals an archetypal confrontation between blameless victim and guilty perpetrator. From this perspective, to deny the validity of recovered memories is to cast aspersions not only on the integrity of the survivors but on the mysterious capacities of the human mind. For the movement's critics, on the other hand, recovered memory is a manifestation of societal hysteria, a quack "solution" that brings legitimate therapies into disrepute. In "Making Monsters" Richard Ofshe, a professor of social psychology at UC- Berkeley, and Ethan Watters, a San Francisco-based journalist, throw down the gantlet to the movement's defenders. In their well-researched, well-argued book, they make a convincing case that recovered memory therapy, rather than revealing actual abuse, often creates false memories of such abuse. Ofshe and Watters persuasively critique the inconsistencies and muddy thinking that beset writings on recovered memories of abuse, from popular manifestoes like "The Courage to Heal" to the recent work of psychiatrist Lenore Terr. And they demonstrate, in chilling detail, the bizarre abuses of the therapeutic process engaged in by psychotherapists themselves. The crux of the debate concerns whether repressed memories of sexual abuse can be "recovered" - remembered for the first time - in treatment with psychotherapists. Ofshe and Watters convincingly demonstrate that the methods employed by some therapists necessarily contaminate and often create the "memories" that emerge. They reject the claim that the human mind is capable of completely repressing numerous traumatic events, maintaining that it goes against what psychologists have discovered about the workings of human memory. Memory of childhood events, they cogently argue, is highly unlikely to take the form of photographic images that are buried and then excavated in pristine form years or decades later. The authors also point out how therapists' misguided use of hypnotic techniques and suggestion create subtle demands for the emergence of false memories. In one of the book's most compelling chapters, they show that patients under hypnosis are just as capable of producing imagined events as factual ones; their fantasies may then be misconstrued as truthful memories by therapist and patient alike. "Making Monsters" is a smart book, bringing scientific rigor and clear thinking to a domain where these qualities have been woefully lacking. It also has the surprising advantage of being a good read; it is that rarest of breeds, a social science page-turner. At times, though, its bracing polemical style is achieved at the expense of a more complex consideration of memory, psychotherapy and experiences of abuse. Take the issue of memory. Ofshe and Watters' presentation of memory research is, in itself, an important contribution, since the theories of memory invoked by recovered memory therapists are for the most part inconsistent and oversimplified. But, because they restrict themselves almost exclusively to empirical research findings, the authors fail to address various conundrums of memory encountered in the therapeutic setting. For instance, the authors suggest that "the memories we continue to remember are those we tend to retell or revisit in our minds." Yet careful and responsible therapists have observed that while sometimes people do not consciously remember traumatic events, they appear to act them out in the behavioral patterns of their lives. This is observed not only with children who suffer traumatic experiences pre-verbally, but in many other ways as well. Transference is precisely what recovered memory therapists simultaneously ignore and exploit in their unexamined roles as persuasive "guides" toward abuse memories. They exploit the patient's willingness to invest them with authority, and they ignore the ways in which their authority influences the patient to comply with their subtle and not-so-subtle agendas. There is no question that "Making Monsters" succeeds in its major goal, to expose the abuses of the recovered memory movement. The case it makes is damning, and it is now up to social critics to try to understand the cultural phenomenon of recovered memory - and up to psychotherapists to clean house. Psychotherapists have to ask themselves some pressing questions: What are the genuinely healing aspects of psychotherapies? How do these go beyond suggestion, persuasion or the simple use of authority? What differentiates bad therapy from good, what kinds of therapy actually help people, and what techniques are especially good for treating what problems? Serious researchers at Ofshe's own university study the psychotherapy process and its outcomes. They are trying to understand psychoanalytic therapy scientifically, and the complex phenomena they study are a far cry from the cartoonish version of Freudian ideas that pervade recovered memory therapy. Psychotherapists need to keep themselves informed of this kind of research, and assess rationally and thoroughly its implications for their work. More generally, how are we to understand why abuse functions for some people as an anchor for their identity? Why do so many people seem to feel abused? What sort of convenient shorthand might the abuse narrative offer people trying to come to grips with diffuse and shameful experiences? Ofshe and Watters point to the identity and sense of belonging that accrues for "survivors" in inpatient settings and therapy groups across the land. Why are certain people in need of this particular mode of belonging? Being a psychotherapist remains an "impossible profession" largely because it requires walking a fine line between skepticism and belief, between the demands of scientific rigor and an openness to surprise. Psychotherapists need to be able to live comfortably with ambiguity and yet strive to distinguish between fact and belief, reality and fantasy. By making a strong case that the phenomenon of recovered memory is specious, Ofshe and Watters have challenged the mental health community. Those who feel equally passionate about the veracity of such memories must step forward and articulate their views, holding themselves to a similarly high standard of evidence.

---

The views and opinions stated within this web page are those of the author or authors which wrote them and may not reflect the views and opinions of the ISP or account user which hosts the web page. The opinions may or may not be those of the Chairman of The Skeptic Tank.

Return to The Skeptic Tank's main Index page.

E-Mail Fredric L. Rice / The Skeptic Tank